Like Father, Like Son — or not?

June 29, 2018
Experience prevails over genetics in some songbird learning, according to research by David Mets

Caption: This is an image of Bengalese finches. Credit: Brainard lab / UCSF

In nature, male Bengalese finches learn their mating songs from their fathers. But how much of that learning is predetermined by genetics? And how much is influenced by experience?

The answers to these questions are important to scientists like former JCC Fellow David Mets (2010-2013) because they can help us understand how other complex behaviors — like human language — are learned.

In Michael Brainard‘s lab at UCSF, Mets noticed that individual birds’ songs varied quite a bit, even when they had learned from the same adult. In a recent study, Mets investigated this phenomenon by removing eggs from nests within 36 hours of being laid — before the developing bird has any nerves with which to hear — and manipulating their learning.

Once hatched, some of the birds were exposed to recordings created from natural birdsongs with varied tempos. These birds grew up to sing like the biological fathers they had never heard. In this case, genetics had a bigger influence on their learning than their experience listening to the recording.

The rest of the birds learned songs from a live unrelated tutor, a foster father. These young birds successfully learned their foster fathers’ songs. Unlike the first group, experience mattered more than genetics.

“This was really exciting because it showed that the experience provided by a live tutor can actually reduce the contribution of genetics to complex behavior like birdsong,” Mets said. “We knew before that live tutors helped birds learn better and faster, but we were surprised to find that this experience can actually override the bird’s genetics.” The study was published in PNAS.

Mets was not always a behavioral geneticist. As a graduate student in Barbara Myer‘s UC Berkeley lab, he studied the mechanisms of genetic recombination during meiosis in C. elegans. For his postdoctoral fellowship, he wanted to study a complex behavioral system that hadn’t been explored very much with genetic approaches. Although he had never taken an ornithology or neuroscience class, studying birds in a neuroscience lab appealed to Mets. “They’re outbred and they represent a complex genetic situation. They’re analogous to humans in their learning.”

His experience working with birds has changed how he thinks about experimental design. C. elegans experiments might last just one or two months, whereas experiments with birds might last two or three years. “There’s a bigger investment in experiments, in terms of time and in the lives of animals.”

Mets plans to continue this research, including studies on the quality of learning and preliminary genetic mapping. “Where in the brain are the father’s genes and early life experience competing for control over song tempo?” he asks. “That’s the next really exciting question.”

  • SHARE